-
(单词翻译:双击或拖选)
AUDIE CORNISH, HOST:
This next story is about a unique relationship between people and a wild animal, a bird called the honeyguide. Like the name suggests, this bird will deliberately1 guide people to honey. NPR's Nell Greenfieldboyce reports a new study by a woman who wanted to understand this unlikely game of follow the leader.
CLAIRE SPOTTISWOODE: I've been obsessed3 with birds since I was about seven, so by then I was already a well-established nerd (laughter).
GREENFIELDBOYCE: The kind of nerd who would go the lectures at her local bird club in Cape4 Town, South Africa. That's where a scientist came to talk about his research on an African bird called the greater honeyguide.
SPOTTISWOODE: And I attended as an 11-year-old child and, of course, was transfixed and enthralled5.
GREENFIELDBOYCE: He explained that this small, grayish bird was legendary6. Stories had long told of how it knew the location of honeybee nests hidden inside of hollow tree trunks. It would tweet at people and flutter in front of them as it flew from tree to tree toward a bees nest. And the lecturer said this wasn't just a folktale. He'd done the first rigorous study.
SPOTTISWOODE: And by following honeyguides, human honey-hunters can really increase their rates of finding bees nest.
GREENFIELDBOYCE: The idea of a wild bird communicating with people in this way seemed almost magical to Spottiswoode, and she learned the birds got something, too. After hunters subdued7 the bees with smoke and hacked8 open the tree to harvest the honey, the birds ate the discarded beeswax, their favorite food.
Spottiswoode grew up to become a researcher with the University of Cambridge in the United Kingdom, and now she's done her own rigorous study of honeyguides in Mozambique. There, honey hunters who follow these birds rely on a distinctive9 call.
SPOTTISWOODE: It goes something like this (imitating honeyguide call) (laughter). So on the face of it, it's a rather unlikely noise.
UNIDENTIFIED MAN #1: (Imitating honeyguide call).
GREENFIELDBOYCE: That's a honey-hunter in Mozambique looking for a guide. And he finds one, or maybe the chattering10 birds finds him. What Spottiswoode wondered is whether that strange call meant anything special to the bird. To find out, she did a study, comparing the birds response to the call with a response to other nonhuman and human sounds like these.
UNIDENTIFIED MAN #2: (Foreign language spoken).
GREENFIELDBOYCE: He's saying, "honeyguide and human" in the local language. It turned out the random11 sounds didn't really appeal to the birds. They'd guide people only about a third of the time. But when they heard...
UNIDENTIFIED MAN #1: (Imitating honeyguide call).
GREENFIELDBOYCE: ...They guided people two-thirds of the time. Overall, making the special call more than tripled a hunter's chance of finding honey. The study is written up in the journal Science, but the dry analysis doesn't really capture how Spottiswoode felt as she talks to a little, wild bird that listened and led her through the trees.
SPOTTISWOODE: I don't think I've ever had as much fun I've had in my life (laughter). Yes, perhaps that shows what a sheltered life I've led, but it was tremendously good fun.
GREENFIELDBOYCE: Honeyguides don't get trained like falcons12. No one breeds them like dogs, yet somehow they responded to specific human calls.
RICHARD WRANGHAM: This is really an extraordinary relationship.
GREENFIELDBOYCE: Richard Wrangham is a biological anthropologist13 at Harvard University. He says, why would birds team up with humans?
WRANGHAM: The critical feature of the relationship is the fact humans have fire as well as axes.
GREENFIELDBOYCE: The tools that let of them harvest honey. That's why Wrangham thinks this collaboration14 might go back more than a million years. The birds may have evolved an innate15 desire to guide people to honey. Still, they're probably not born knowing what human sounds to listen for. That's because people in different parts of Africa called the birds in different ways. For example, in Tanzania, people whistle at that the birds.
UNIDENTIFIED PERSON: (Whistling).
GREENFIELDBOYCE: So how does a wild bird learn which human sounds matter - maybe from other birds, but scientists aren't sure. Nell Greenfieldboyce, NPR News.
点击收听单词发音
1 deliberately | |
adv.审慎地;蓄意地;故意地 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
2 byline | |
n.署名;v.署名 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
3 obsessed | |
adj.心神不宁的,鬼迷心窍的,沉迷的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
4 cape | |
n.海角,岬;披肩,短披风 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
5 enthralled | |
迷住,吸引住( enthrall的过去式和过去分词 ); 使感到非常愉快 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
6 legendary | |
adj.传奇(中)的,闻名遐迩的;n.传奇(文学) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
7 subdued | |
adj. 屈服的,柔和的,减弱的 动词subdue的过去式和过去分词 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
8 hacked | |
生气 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
9 distinctive | |
adj.特别的,有特色的,与众不同的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
10 chattering | |
n. (机器振动发出的)咔嗒声,(鸟等)鸣,啁啾 adj. 喋喋不休的,啾啾声的 动词chatter的现在分词形式 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
11 random | |
adj.随机的;任意的;n.偶然的(或随便的)行动 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
12 falcons | |
n.猎鹰( falcon的名词复数 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
13 anthropologist | |
n.人类学家,人类学者 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
14 collaboration | |
n.合作,协作;勾结 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
15 innate | |
adj.天生的,固有的,天赋的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|