-
(单词翻译:双击或拖选)
17.The primary purpose of the passage is to
(A) reconcile two opposing theories
(B) compare two different explanations for a phe-
nomenon
(C) describe experimental research that appears to
support an unpopular theory
(D) present evidence that may help to resolve an
apparent contradiction
(E) describe a hypothesis that has cause a con-
troversy
18.It can be inferred from the passage that the author
would most likely describe the "additional evidence"
(line 12) provided by experiments with adrenergic
antagonists1 as
(A) revolutionary
(B) disappointing
(C) incomplete
(D) unexpected
(E) corroborative2
19.The passage provides information about which of the
following topics?
(A) The mechanism3 by which glucose4 affects memory
storage
(B) The evidence that prompted scientist to test the
effects of adrenaline on memory regulation
(C) The reason that the effects of glucose on memory
were tested
(D) The ways that memory storage modifies the struc-
ture of the brain
(E) The kinds of training used to test memory enhance-
ment in rats
20.The author refers to the results of the experiment using
adrenergic antagonists as "negative findings" (line 13)
most likely because the adrenergic antagonists
(A) failed to disrupt adrenaline’s effect on memory
(B) did not affect glucose’s ability to enhance memory.
(C) did not block adrenaline’s ability to increase blood
glucose levels
(D) only partially5 affected6 adrenaline’s ability to
enhance memory
(E) disrupted both adrenaline’s and glucose’s effect
on memory
The age at which young children begin to make moral
discriminations about harmful actions committed against
themselves or others has been the focus of recent research
into the moral development of children. Until recently,
(5)child psychologists supported pioneer developmentalist Jean.
Piaget in his hypothesis that because of their immaturity7,
children under age seven do not take into account the inten-
tions of a person committing accidental or deliberate harm,
but rather simply assign punishment for transgressions9 on
(10)the basis of the magnitude of the negative consequences
caused. According to Piaget, children under age seven
occupy the first stage of moral development, which is char-
acterized by moral absolutism (rules made by authorities
must be obeyed) and imminent10 justice (if rules are broken,
(15)punishment will be meted11 out). Until young children mature,
their moral judgments12 are based entirely13 on the effect
rather than the cause of a transgression8. However, in recent
research, Keasey found that six- year-old children not only
distinguish between accidental and intentional14 harm, but
(20)also judge intentional harm as naughtier, regardless of the
amount of damage produced. Both of these findings seem
to indicate that children, at an earlier age than Piaget
claimed, advance into the second stage of moral develop-
ment, moral autonomy, in which they accept social rules
(25)but view them as more arbitrary than do children in the
first stage.
Keasey’s research raises two key questions for develop-
mental psychologists about children under age seven: do
they recognize justifications15 for harmful actions, and do
(30)they make distinctions between harmful acts that are pre-
ventable and those acts that have unforeseen harmful con-
sequences? Studies indicate that justifications excusing
harmful actions might include public duty,self-defense, and
provocation16. For example, Nesdale and Rule concluded that
(35)children were capable of considering whether or not an
aggressor’s action was justified17 by public duty: five year
olds reacted very differently to "Bonnie wrecks18 Ann’s
pretend house" depending on whether Bonnie did it "so
somebody won’t fall over it" or because Bonnie wanted "to
(40)make Ann feel bad."Thus, a child of five begins to under-
stand that certain harmful actions, though intentional, can
be justified; the constraints19 of moral absolutism no longer
solely20 guide their judgments.
Psychologists have determined21 that during kindergarten
(45)children learn to make subtle distinctions involving harm.
Darley observed that among acts involving unintentional
harm, six-year-old children just entering kindergarten could
not differentiate22 between foreseeable, and thus preventable,
harm and unforeseeable harm for which the perpetrator
(50)cannot be blamed. Seven months later, however, Darley
found that these same children could make both distinc-
tions, thus demonstrating that they had become morally
autonomous23.
21.Which of the following best describes the passage as
a whole?
(A) An outline for future research
(B) An expanded definition of commonly misunder-
stood terms
(C) An analysis of a dispute between two theorists
(D) A discussion of research findings in an ongoing24
inquiry25
(E) A confirmation26 of an established authority’s theory
22.According to the passage, Darley found that after seven
months of kindergarten six year olds acquired which of
the following abilities?
(A) Differentiating between foreseeable and unforesee-
able harm
(B) Identifying with the perpetrator of a harmful action
(C) Justifying harmful actions that result from provo-
cation
(D) Evaluating the magnitude of negative consequences
resulting from the breaking of rules
(E) Recognizing the difference between moral absolu-
tism and moral autonomy
(A) reconcile two opposing theories
(B) compare two different explanations for a phe-
nomenon
(C) describe experimental research that appears to
support an unpopular theory
(D) present evidence that may help to resolve an
apparent contradiction
(E) describe a hypothesis that has cause a con-
troversy
18.It can be inferred from the passage that the author
would most likely describe the "additional evidence"
(line 12) provided by experiments with adrenergic
antagonists1 as
(A) revolutionary
(B) disappointing
(C) incomplete
(D) unexpected
(E) corroborative2
19.The passage provides information about which of the
following topics?
(A) The mechanism3 by which glucose4 affects memory
storage
(B) The evidence that prompted scientist to test the
effects of adrenaline on memory regulation
(C) The reason that the effects of glucose on memory
were tested
(D) The ways that memory storage modifies the struc-
ture of the brain
(E) The kinds of training used to test memory enhance-
ment in rats
20.The author refers to the results of the experiment using
adrenergic antagonists as "negative findings" (line 13)
most likely because the adrenergic antagonists
(A) failed to disrupt adrenaline’s effect on memory
(B) did not affect glucose’s ability to enhance memory.
(C) did not block adrenaline’s ability to increase blood
glucose levels
(D) only partially5 affected6 adrenaline’s ability to
enhance memory
(E) disrupted both adrenaline’s and glucose’s effect
on memory
The age at which young children begin to make moral
discriminations about harmful actions committed against
themselves or others has been the focus of recent research
into the moral development of children. Until recently,
(5)child psychologists supported pioneer developmentalist Jean.
Piaget in his hypothesis that because of their immaturity7,
children under age seven do not take into account the inten-
tions of a person committing accidental or deliberate harm,
but rather simply assign punishment for transgressions9 on
(10)the basis of the magnitude of the negative consequences
caused. According to Piaget, children under age seven
occupy the first stage of moral development, which is char-
acterized by moral absolutism (rules made by authorities
must be obeyed) and imminent10 justice (if rules are broken,
(15)punishment will be meted11 out). Until young children mature,
their moral judgments12 are based entirely13 on the effect
rather than the cause of a transgression8. However, in recent
research, Keasey found that six- year-old children not only
distinguish between accidental and intentional14 harm, but
(20)also judge intentional harm as naughtier, regardless of the
amount of damage produced. Both of these findings seem
to indicate that children, at an earlier age than Piaget
claimed, advance into the second stage of moral develop-
ment, moral autonomy, in which they accept social rules
(25)but view them as more arbitrary than do children in the
first stage.
Keasey’s research raises two key questions for develop-
mental psychologists about children under age seven: do
they recognize justifications15 for harmful actions, and do
(30)they make distinctions between harmful acts that are pre-
ventable and those acts that have unforeseen harmful con-
sequences? Studies indicate that justifications excusing
harmful actions might include public duty,self-defense, and
provocation16. For example, Nesdale and Rule concluded that
(35)children were capable of considering whether or not an
aggressor’s action was justified17 by public duty: five year
olds reacted very differently to "Bonnie wrecks18 Ann’s
pretend house" depending on whether Bonnie did it "so
somebody won’t fall over it" or because Bonnie wanted "to
(40)make Ann feel bad."Thus, a child of five begins to under-
stand that certain harmful actions, though intentional, can
be justified; the constraints19 of moral absolutism no longer
solely20 guide their judgments.
Psychologists have determined21 that during kindergarten
(45)children learn to make subtle distinctions involving harm.
Darley observed that among acts involving unintentional
harm, six-year-old children just entering kindergarten could
not differentiate22 between foreseeable, and thus preventable,
harm and unforeseeable harm for which the perpetrator
(50)cannot be blamed. Seven months later, however, Darley
found that these same children could make both distinc-
tions, thus demonstrating that they had become morally
autonomous23.
21.Which of the following best describes the passage as
a whole?
(A) An outline for future research
(B) An expanded definition of commonly misunder-
stood terms
(C) An analysis of a dispute between two theorists
(D) A discussion of research findings in an ongoing24
inquiry25
(E) A confirmation26 of an established authority’s theory
22.According to the passage, Darley found that after seven
months of kindergarten six year olds acquired which of
the following abilities?
(A) Differentiating between foreseeable and unforesee-
able harm
(B) Identifying with the perpetrator of a harmful action
(C) Justifying harmful actions that result from provo-
cation
(D) Evaluating the magnitude of negative consequences
resulting from the breaking of rules
(E) Recognizing the difference between moral absolu-
tism and moral autonomy
点击收听单词发音
1 antagonists | |
对立[对抗] 者,对手,敌手( antagonist的名词复数 ); 对抗肌; 对抗药 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
2 corroborative | |
adj.确证(性)的,确凿的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
3 mechanism | |
n.机械装置;机构,结构 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
4 glucose | |
n.葡萄糖 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
5 partially | |
adv.部分地,从某些方面讲 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
6 affected | |
adj.不自然的,假装的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
7 immaturity | |
n.不成熟;未充分成长;未成熟;粗糙 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
8 transgression | |
n.违背;犯规;罪过 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
9 transgressions | |
n.违反,违法,罪过( transgression的名词复数 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
10 imminent | |
adj.即将发生的,临近的,逼近的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
11 meted | |
v.(对某人)施以,给予(处罚等)( mete的过去式和过去分词 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
12 judgments | |
判断( judgment的名词复数 ); 鉴定; 评价; 审判 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
13 entirely | |
ad.全部地,完整地;完全地,彻底地 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
14 intentional | |
adj.故意的,有意(识)的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
15 justifications | |
正当的理由,辩解的理由( justification的名词复数 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
16 provocation | |
n.激怒,刺激,挑拨,挑衅的事物,激怒的原因 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
17 justified | |
a.正当的,有理的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
18 wrecks | |
n.沉船( wreck的名词复数 );(事故中)遭严重毁坏的汽车(或飞机等);(身体或精神上)受到严重损伤的人;状况非常糟糕的车辆(或建筑物等)v.毁坏[毁灭]某物( wreck的第三人称单数 );使(船舶)失事,使遇难,使下沉 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
19 constraints | |
强制( constraint的名词复数 ); 限制; 约束 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
20 solely | |
adv.仅仅,唯一地 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
21 determined | |
adj.坚定的;有决心的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
22 differentiate | |
vi.(between)区分;vt.区别;使不同 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
23 autonomous | |
adj.自治的;独立的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
24 ongoing | |
adj.进行中的,前进的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
25 inquiry | |
n.打听,询问,调查,查问 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
26 confirmation | |
n.证实,确认,批准 | |
参考例句: |
|
|